You are here

Contrary to popular belief, a dog’s breed will not predict behaviour

By AFP - May 26,2022 - Last updated at May 26,2022

 

WASHINGTON — They’re well-known stereotypes: Rottweilers and pit bulls are aggressive, while Labradors and golden retrievers are extra friendly.

But a genetic study published recently in the journal Science involving more than 2,000 dogs paired with 200,000 survey answers from owners demonstrates that the widespread assumptions are largely unfounded.

To be sure, many behavioural traits can be inherited — but the modern concept of breed offers only partial predictive value for most types of behaviour — and almost none whatsoever for how affectionate a dog will be, or conversely, how quick to anger.

“While genetics plays a role in the personality of any individual dog, specific dog breed is not a good predictor of those traits,” said senior author Elinor Karlsson, of UMass Chan and the Broad Institute of MIT and Harvard.

“What we found is that the defining criteria of a golden retriever are its physical characteristics — the shape of its ears, the colour and quality of its fur, its size — not whether it is friendly,” she added.

Lead author Kathleen Morrill explained that understanding the relationship between breeds and behaviour could be the first step in understanding the genes responsible for psychiatric conditions in humans, like obsessive disorders.

“Although we can’t really ask a dog themselves about their problems or thoughts or anxieties, we do know that dogs lead rich emotional lives and experience disorders that manifests in their behaviour,” she said on a press call. 

 

Implications for legislation

 

The team sequenced the DNA of 2,155 purebred and mixed-breed dogs to search for common genetic variations that could predict behaviour, and combined this info with surveys from 18,385 pet-owner surveys from Darwin’s Ark.

The site is an open-source database of owner-reported canine traits and behaviours. 

Because existing stereotypes are so powerful, the team designed their questionnaires to account for owner bias. 

They established standard definitions for reporting traits such as biddability (dog response to human direction), dog-human sociability (how comfortable dogs are with people, including strangers), and toy-directed motor patterns (how interested they are in toys).

Physical and aesthetic traits were also surveyed.

In all, Karlsson and Morrill found 11 locations on the dog genome associated with behaviour differences, including biddability, retrieving, pointing at a target and howling.

Among these behaviours, breed did play some role — for example, beagles and bloodhounds tend to howl more, border collies are biddable, and Shiba Inus are far less so.

However, there were always exceptions to the rule. 

For example, even though Labs had the lowest propensity for howling, eight per cent still did. While 90 per cent of greyhounds didn’t bury their toys, three per cent did frequently.

“When we looked at this factor that we called agonistic threshold, which included a lot of questions about whether people’s dogs reacted aggressively to things, we weren’t seeing an effect of breed ancestry,” Karlsson added.

Overall, breed explained just nine per cent of variation in behaviour, with age a better predictor of some traits, like toy play. Physical traits, however, were five times more likely to be predicted by breed than behaviour was.

The idea runs counter to widespread assumptions that have informed legislation. For example, Britain has banned pit bull terriers, as have many US cities.

 

Human disorders

 

Prior to the 1800s, dogs were primarily selected for functional roles such as hunting, guarding and herding, the team said in their paper. 

“By contrast, the modern dog breed, emphasising confirmation to physical ideals and purity of lineage, is a Victorian invention,” they wrote. 

Modern breeds carry genetic variations of their ancient predecessors, but not at the same frequencies — explaining the behaviour divergence within breeds.

The next steps, said Morill, would be digging more into compulsive behaviours in dogs, and connections to human obsessive-compulsive disorder.

One intriguing finding was that dog sociability towards humans was “incredibly heritable in dogs”, even though it wasn’t breed dependent. 

The team found a location in dog DNA that could explain four per cent of the sociability differences between individuals — and that location corresponds to an area of the human genome responsible for long-term memory formation.

“It could be that understanding human sociability in dogs helps us understand how brains develop and learn. So we’re kind of just scratching the surface,” said Morill.

up
13 users have voted.


Newsletter

Get top stories and blog posts emailed to you each day.

PDF